Constructivism (philosophy of education)

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected from Constructivist theory)

Jean Piaget constructed the theory of cognitive development, which describes how children represent and reason about the world.[1][2]

Constructivism in education is a theory that suggests that learners do not passively acquire knowledge through direct instruction. Instead, they construct their understanding through experiences and social interaction, integrating new information with their existing knowledge. This theory originates from Swiss developmental psychologist Jean Piaget's theory of cognitive development.

Background[edit]

Constructivism in education is rooted in epistemology, a theory of knowledge concerned with the logical categories of knowledge and its justification.[3] It acknowledges that learners bring prior knowledge and experiences shaped by their social and cultural environment and that learning is a process of students "constructing" knowledge based on their experiences. While Behaviorism focuses on understanding what students are doing, constructivism emphasizes the importance of understanding what students are thinking and how to enrich their thinking.[4]

Constructivism in educational psychology can be attributed to the work of Jean Piaget (1896–1980) and his theory of cognitive development. Piaget's focus was on how humans make meaning by integrating experiences with ideas, emphasizing human development as distinct from external influences[5] Another influential figure, Lev Vygotsky (1896-1934), emphasized the importance of sociocultural learning in his theory of social constructivism, highlighting how interactions with adults, peers, and cognitive tools contribute to the formation of mental constructs. Building upon Vygotsky's work, Jerome Bruner and other educational psychologists introduced the concept of instructional scaffolding, where the learning environment provides support that is gradually removed as learners internalize the knowledge.[4]

Views more focused on human development within the social sphere include the sociocultural or socio-historical perspective of Lev Vygotsky and the situated cognition perspectives of Mikhail Bakhtin, Jean Lave, and Etienne Wenger. [6]Additionally, the works of Brown, Collins, and Duguid,[7] as well as Newman, Griffin, Cole,[8] and Barbara Rogoff[9].

The concept of constructivism has impacted a number of disciplines, including psychology, sociology, education, and the history of science.[10] In its early stages, constructivism focused on the relationship between human experiences and their reflexes or behavior patterns. Piaget referred to these systems of knowledge "schemes."

Piaget's theory of constructivist learning has significantly influenced learning theories and teaching methods in education. It serves as a foundational concept in education reform movements within cognitive science and neuroscience.[11]

Overview[edit]

The formalization of constructivism from a within-the-human perspective is commonly credited to Jean Piaget. Piaget described the mechanisms by which information from the environment and ideas from the individual interact to form internalized structures developed by learners. He identified processes of assimilation and accommodation as crucial in this interaction, as individuals construct new knowledge from their experiences.

When individuals assimilate new information, they integrate it into their existing framework without altering that framework. This can happen when their experiences align with their internal view of the world, but it can also occur if they fail to update a flawed understanding. Accommodation is the process of adjusting one's mental representation of the external world to fit new experiences. It can be understood as the mechanism by which failure leads to learning.

It is important to note that constructivism is not a specific pedagogy, but rather a theory explaining how learning occurs, regardless of the learning environment. However, constructivism is often associated with pedagogic approaches that promote active learning, or learning by doing. While there is much enthusiasm for constructivism as a design strategy, some experts believe that it is more of a philosophical framework than a theory that can precisely describe instruction or prescribe design strategies.[12]: 4 

Constructivist Pedagogy[edit]

The nature of the learner[edit]

Social constructivism recognizes and embraces the individuality and complexity of each learner, actively encouraging and rewarding it as a vital component of the learning process.[13]

The importance of the background and culture of the learner[edit]

Social constructivism, also known as socioculturalism, emphasizes the role of an individual's background, culture, and worldview in shaping their understanding of truth. According to this theory, learners inherit historical developments and symbol systems from their culture and continue to learn and develop these throughout their lives. This approach highlights the significance of a learner's social interactions with knowledgeable members of society. It suggests that without such interactions, it is challenging to grasp the social meaning of important symbol systems and learn how to effectively use them. Social constructivism also points out that young children develop their thinking abilities through interactions with peers, adults, and the physical world. Therefore, it is essential to consider the learner's background and culture throughout the learning process, as these factors help shape the knowledge and truth that the learner acquires.[13]

Responsibility for learning[edit]

Social constructivism emphasizes the importance of the student being actively involved in the learning process, unlike previous educational viewpoints where the responsibility rested with the instructor to teach and where the learner played a passive, receptive role. Von Glasersfeld (1989) emphasized that learners construct their own understanding and that they do not simply mirror and reflect what they read. Learners look for meaning and will try to find regularity and order in the events of the world even in the absence of full or complete information.[14]

The motivation for learning[edit]

When considering students' learning, it is essential to take into account their motivation and confidence. According to Von Glasersfeld, a student's motivation to learn is strongly influenced by their belief in their potential for learning[14] This belief is shaped by their past experiences of successfully mastering problems, which is more influential than external acknowledgment and motivation.[15] This idea aligns with Vygotsky's concept of the "zone of proximal development," where students are challenged at a level slightly above their current development. By successfully completing challenging tasks, students build confidence and motivation to take on even more complex challenges.[16]

The role of the instructor[edit]

Instructors as facilitators[edit]

According to the social constructivist approach, instructors are expected to adapt to the role of facilitators rather than traditional teachers.[17] While a teacher teacher gives a didactic lecture that covers the subject matter, a facilitator assists the student in developing their own understanding of the content. This shift in roles places the focus on the student's active involvement in the learning process, as opposed to the instructor and the content itself. [18]

As a result, a facilitator requires a different set of skills compared to a teacher.[19] For instance, a teacher imparts information, whereas a facilitator encourages questions; a teacher leads from the front, while a facilitator provides support from the background; and a teacher delivers answers based on a set curriculum, whereas a facilitator offers guidance and creates an environment for the learner to form their own conclusions. Furthermore, a teacher typically engages in a monologue, whereas a facilitator maintains an ongoing dialogue with the learners.[20]

Additionally, a facilitator should be able to dynamically adapt the learning experience by taking the lead in guiding the experience to align with the learners' interests and needs in order to create value.

The learning environment should be created in a way that both supports and challenges the student's thinking[21] While it is advocated to give the student ownership of the problem and solution process, it is not the case that any and all activities or solutions are adequate. The critical goal is to support the student in developing effective thinking skills.

Relationship between instructor and students[edit]

In the social constructivist viewpoint, the role of the facilitator involves both the instructor and the students being actively engaged in learning from each other.[22] This dynamic interaction requires that the instructor's culture, values, and background play a significant part in shaping the learning experience. Students compare their own thoughts with those of the instructor and their peers, leading to the development of a new, socially validated understanding of the subject matter. [23] The task or problem serves as the interface between the instructor and the student, creating a dynamic interaction. As a result, both students and instructors need to develop an awareness of each other's viewpoints and consider their own beliefs, standards, and values, making the learning experience both subjective and objective at the same time.[24]

Several studies highlight the significance of mentoring in the learning process.[7][25] The social constructivist model underscores the importance of the relationship between the student and the instructor in facilitating learning.

Interactive learning can be facilitated through various approaches such as reciprocal teaching, peer collaboration, cognitive apprenticeship, problem-based instruction, Anchored Instruction, and other methods that involve collaborative learning.

Learning is an active process[edit]

Social constructivism, which is strongly influenced by Vygotsky's work, proposes that knowledge is initially built within a social setting and is then taken in by individuals.[26] According to social constructivists, the act of sharing individual viewpoints, known as collaborative elaboration, leads to learners jointly constructing understanding that would not be achievable on their own.[27][28]

Social constructivist scholars view learning as an active process in which students are encouraged to discover principles, concepts, and facts independently. Therefore, it is crucial to promote speculation and intuitive thinking in students.[29]

According to other constructivist scholars, individuals create meanings through their interactions with each other and the environment they inhabit.[30] Knowledge is created by people and is shaped by social and cultural influences.[15][31] McMahon (1997) also emphasizes the social nature of learning, stating that it is not solely a mental process or a result of external factors shaping behavior. Instead, meaningful learning occurs when individuals participate in social activities.[23]

According to Vygotsky (1978), an important aspect of intellectual development is the convergence of speech and practical activity. He emphasized that as children engage in practical activities, they construct meaning on an individual level, and through speech, they connect this meaning to their culture and the interpersonal world they share with others.[16]

Collaboration among learners[edit]

Another tenet of social constructivism is that collaboration among individuals with diverse skills and backgrounds is essential for developing a comprehensive understanding of a particular subject or field.[32]

In some social constructivist models, there is an emphasis on the importance of collaboration among learners, which contrasts with traditional competitive approaches. One concept from Vygotsky that is particularly relevant to peer collaboration is the zone of proximal development. This is defined as the gap between a learner's actual developmental level, determined by independent problem-solving, and the level of potential development, determined through problem-solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers. It differs from Piaget's fixed biological stages of development. Through a process called "scaffolding," a learner can be extended beyond the limitations of physical maturation, allowing the development process to catch up to the learning process.[16]

When students present and teach new material to their peers, it fosters a non-linear process of collective knowledge construction.

The importance of context[edit]

The social constructivist paradigm emphasizes that the environment in which learning takes place plays a crucial role in the learning process.[23]

The concept of the learner as an active processor is based on the idea that there are no universal learning laws that apply to all domains.[21]: 208  When individuals possess decontextualized knowledge, they may struggle to apply their understanding to real-world tasks. This is due to the lack of engagement with the concept in its complex, real-world environment, as well as the absence of experience with the intricate interrelationships that influence the application of the concept.[32]

One concept within social constructivism is authentic or situated learning, which involves students participating in activities directly related to the practical application of their learning within a culture similar to the real-world setting. Cognitive apprenticeship is a suggested effective model of constructivist learning that aims to immerse students in authentic practices through activity and social interaction, similar to the successful methods used in craft apprenticeship.[[29]: 25 

Holt and Willard-Holt (2000) highlight the concept of dynamic assessment, which offers a distinct approach to evaluating learners compared to traditional tests. Dynamic assessment extends the interactive nature of learning to the assessment process, emphasizing interaction between the assessor and the learner. It involves a dialogue between the assessor and the learner to understand the current performance level on a task and explore ways to improve future performance. This approach views assessment and learning as interconnected processes, rather than separate entities.[22]

According to this viewpoint, instructors should approach assessment as an ongoing and interactive process that evaluates the learner's achievements, the quality of the learning experience, and course materials. The feedback generated by the assessment process is crucial for driving further development.

The selection, scope, and sequencing of the subject matter[edit]

Knowledge should be discovered as an integrated whole[edit]

The organization of knowledge should prioritize integration over division into separate subjects or compartments.[21][23]

This again emphasizes the significance of presenting learning within a specific context.[7] The world in which learners operate is not divided into separate subjects but rather comprises a complex array of facts, problems, dimensions, and perceptions.[29]

Engaging and challenging the student[edit]

Students benefit from being challenged with tasks that require them to apply skills and knowledge slightly beyond their current level of mastery. This approach can help to maintain their motivation and build on past achievements to boost their confidence.[19] This is in line with Vygotsky's zone of proximal development, which refers to the gap between a person's current level of ability and their potential level of development under the guidance of adults or more capable peers.[16]

Vygotsky (1978) argued that effective instruction should be slightly ahead of a learner's current developmental stage. By doing so, instruction can stimulate the development of a range of functions that are in the learner's zone of proximal development. This highlights the crucial role of instruction in fostering development.[16]

In order to effectively engage and challenge students, it is important that the tasks and learning environment mirror the complexity of the real-world environment in which the students are expected to operate upon completing their education. Students should not only take ownership of the learning and problem-solving process but also take ownership of the problems themselves.[33]

When it comes to organizing subject matter, the constructivist perspective suggests that the fundamental principles of any subject can be taught to anyone at any point, in some capacity. [32] This approach entails introducing the foundational concepts that makeup topics or subject areas initially and then consistently revisiting and expanding on these ideas.

Instructors should recognize that while they are given a set curriculum to follow, they inevitably personalize it to reflect their own beliefs, thoughts, and emotions about the subject matter and their students. As a result, the learning experience becomes a collaborative effort, influenced by the emotions and life experiences of all involved. It's important to consider the student's motivation as central to the learning process.[7][29]

The structuredness of the learning process[edit]

It is important to achieve the right balance between the degree of structure and flexibility that is built into the learning process. Savery (1994) contends that the more structured the learning environment, the harder it is for the learners to construct meaning based on their conceptual understandings. A facilitator should structure the learning experience just enough to make sure that the students get clear guidance and parameters within which to achieve the learning objectives, yet the learning experience should be open and free enough to allow for the learners to discover, enjoy, interact and arrive at their own, socially verified version of truth.[24]

Teaching Techniques[edit]

A few strategies for cooperative learning include:

  • Reciprocal Questioning: students work together to ask and answer questions
  • Jigsaw Classroom: students become "experts" on one part of a group project and teach it to the others in their group
  • Structured Controversies: Students work together to research a particular controversy[34]

The Harkness discussion method[edit]

It is called the "Harkness" discussion method because it was developed at Phillips Exeter Academy with funds donated in the 1930s by Edward Harkness. This is also named after the Harkness table and involves students seated in a circle, motivating and controlling their own discussion. The teacher acts as little as possible. Perhaps the teacher's only function is to observe, although they might begin or shift or even direct a discussion. The students get it rolling, direct it, and focus it. They act as a team, cooperatively, to make it work. They all participate, but not in a competitive way. Rather, they all share in the responsibility and the goals, much as any members share in any team sport. Although the goals of any discussion will change depending upon what's under discussion, some goals will always be the same: to illuminate the subject, to unravel its mysteries, to interpret and share and learn from other points of view, to piece together the puzzle using everyone's contribution. Discussion skills are important. Everyone must be aware of how to get this discussion rolling and keep it rolling and interesting. Just as in any sport, a number of skills are necessary to work on and use at appropriate times. Everyone is expected to contribute by using these skills.[citation needed]

Pedagogies based on constructivism[edit]

Various approaches in pedagogy derive from constructivist theory. They usually suggest that learning is accomplished best using a hands-on approach. Learners learn by experimentation, and not by being told what will happen, and are left to make their own inferences, discoveries and conclusions.

In adult learning[edit]

Constructivist ideas have been used to inform adult education. Current trends in higher education push for more "active learning" teaching approaches which are often based on constructivist views.[citation needed]

Approaches based on constructivism stress the importance of mechanisms for mutual planning, diagnosis of learner needs and interests, cooperative learning climate, sequential activities for achieving the objectives, formulation of learning objectives based on the diagnosed needs and interests. While adult learning often stresses the importance of personal relevance of the content, involvement of the learner in the process, and deeper understanding of underlying concepts, all of these are principles that may benefit learners of all ages as even children connect their every day experiences to what they learn.[citation needed]

Supporting research and evidence[edit]

Hmelo-Silver, Duncan, & Chinn cite several studies supporting the success of the constructivist problem-based and inquiry learning methods. For example, they describe a project called GenScope, an inquiry-based science software application. Students using the GenScope software showed significant gains over the control groups, with the largest gains shown in students from basic courses.[35]

Hmelo-Silver et al. also cite a large study by Geier on the effectiveness of inquiry-based science for middle school students, as demonstrated by their performance on high-stakes standardized tests. The improvement was 14% for the first cohort of students and 13% for the second cohort. This study also found that inquiry-based teaching methods greatly reduced the achievement gap for African-American students.[35]

Guthrie et al. (2004) compared three instructional methods for third-grade reading: a traditional approach, a strategies instruction only approach, and an approach with strategies instruction and constructivist motivation techniques including student choices, collaboration, and hands-on activities. The constructivist approach, called CORI (Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction), resulted in better student reading comprehension, cognitive strategies, and motivation.[36]

Jong Suk Kim found that using constructivist teaching methods for 6th graders resulted in better student achievement than traditional teaching methods. This study also found that students preferred constructivist methods over traditional ones. However, Kim did not find any difference in student self-concept or learning strategies between those taught by constructivist or traditional methods.[37]

Doğru and Kalender compared science classrooms using traditional teacher-centered approaches to those using student-centered, constructivist methods. In their initial test of student performance immediately following the lessons, they found no significant difference between traditional and constructivist methods. However, in the follow-up assessment 15 days later, students who learned through constructivist methods showed better retention of knowledge than those who learned through traditional methods.[38]

Criticism[edit]

Several cognitive psychologists and educators have questioned the central claims of constructivism. It is argued that constructivist theories are misleading or contradict known findings.[39][40][41][42][43] Matthews (1993) attempts to sketch the influence of constructivism in current mathematics and science education, aiming to indicate how pervasive Aristotle's empiricist epistemology is within it and what problems constructivism faces on that account.[44]

In the neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development it is maintained that learning at any age depends upon the processing and representational resources available at this particular age. That is, it is maintained that if the requirements of the concept to be understood exceeds the available processing efficiency and working memory resources then the concept is by definition not learnable. This attitude toward learning impedes the learning from understanding essential theoretical concepts or, in other words, reasoning.[45] Therefore, no matter how active a child is during learning, to learn the child must operate in a learning environment that meets the developmental and individual learning constraints that are characteristic for the child's age and this child's possible deviations from her age's norm. If this condition is not met, construction goes astray.[46][47]

Several educators have also questioned the effectiveness of this approach toward instructional design, especially as it applies to the development of instruction for novices.[39][48] While some constructivists argue that "learning by doing" enhances learning, critics of this instructional strategy argue that little empirical evidence exists to support this statement given novice learners.[39][48] Sweller and his colleagues argue that novices do not possess the underlying mental models, or "schemas" necessary for "learning by doing".[49] Indeed, Mayer (2004) reviewed the literature and found that fifty years of empirical data do not support using the constructivist teaching technique of pure discovery; in those situations requiring discovery, he argues for the use of guided discovery instead.[48]

Mayer (2004) argues that not all teaching techniques based on constructivism are efficient or effective for all learners, suggesting many educators misapply constructivism to use teaching techniques that require learners to be behaviorally active. He describes this inappropriate use of constructivism as the "constructivist teaching fallacy". "I refer to this interpretation as the constructivist teaching fallacy because it equates active learning with active teaching."[48]: 15  Instead Mayer proposes learners should be "cognitively active" during learning and that instructors use "guided practice."

In contrast, Kirschner et al. (2006)[39] describe constructivist teaching methods as "unguided methods of instruction." They suggest more structured learning activities for learners with little to no prior knowledge. Slezak states that constructivism "is an example of fashionable but thoroughly problematic doctrines that can have little benefit for practical pedagogy or teacher education."[50] Similar views have been stated by Meyer,[51] Boden, Quale and others.

Kirschner et al. group a number of learning theories together (Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based learning) and stated that highly scaffolded constructivist methods like problem-based learning and inquiry learning are ineffective.[39] Kirschner et al. described several research studies that were favorable to problem-based learning given learners were provided some level of guidance and support.[39]

A rebuttal to the criticisms of Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark[edit]

While there are critics of the Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark[39] article, Sweller and his associates have written in their articles about:

  1. instructional designs for producing procedural learning (learning as behavior change);[49]
  2. their grouping of seemingly disparate learning theories[39] and;
  3. a continuum of guidance beginning with worked examples that may be followed by practice, or transitioned to practice[52] (Renkl, Atkinson, Maier, and Staley, 2002)

Kirschner et al. (2006) describe worked examples as an instructional design solution for procedural learning.[39] Clark, Nguyen, and Sweller (2006) describe this as a very effective, empirically validated method of teaching learners procedural skill acquisition. Evidence for learning by studying worked-examples, is known as the worked-example effect and has been found to be useful in many domains (e.g. music, chess, athletics)[53] concept mapping,[54] geometry,[55] physics, mathematics, or programming.[56]

Kirschner et al. (2006)[39] describe why they group a series of seemingly disparate learning theories (Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based learning). The reasoning for this grouping is because each learning theory promotes the same constructivist teaching technique—"learning by doing." While they argue "learning by doing" is useful for more knowledgeable learners, they argue this teaching technique is not useful for novices. Mayer states that it promotes behavioral activity too early in the learning process, when learners should be cognitively active.[48]

In addition, Sweller and his associates describe a continuum of guidance, starting with worked examples to slowly fade guidance. This continuum of faded guidance has been tested empirically to produce a series of learning effects: the worked-example effect,[57] the guidance fading effect,[58] and the expertise-reversal effect.[52]

Criticism of discovery-based teaching techniques[edit]

After a half century of advocacy associated with instruction using minimal guidance, there appears no body of research supporting the technique. In so far as there is any evidence from controlled studies, it almost uniformly supports direct, strong instructional guidance rather constructivist-based minimal guidance during the instruction of novice to intermediate learners. Even for students with considerable prior knowledge, strong guidance while learning is most often found to be equally effective as unguided approaches. Not only is unguided instruction normally less effective; there is also evidence that it may have negative results when students acquire misconceptions or incomplete or disorganized knowledge

— Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work: An Analysis of the Failure of Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based Teaching by Kirschner, Sweller, Clark[39]

Mayer (2004) argues against discovery-based teaching techniques and provides an extensive review to support this argument. Mayer's arguments are against pure discovery, and are not specifically aimed at constructivism: "Nothing in this article should be construed as arguing against the view of learning as knowledge construction or against using hands-on inquiry or group discussion that promotes the process of knowledge construction in learners. The main conclusion I draw from the three research literatures I have reviewed is that it would be a mistake to interpret the current constructivist view of learning as a rationale for reviving pure discovery as a method of instruction."[48]

Mayer's concern is how one applies discovery-based teaching techniques. He provides empirical research as evidence that discovery-based teaching techniques are inadequate. Here he cites this literature and makes his point "For example, a recent replication is research showing that students learn to become better at solving mathematics problems when they study worked-out examples rather than when they solely engage in hands-on problem solving.[59] Today's proponents of discovery methods, who claim to draw their support from constructivist philosophy, are making inroads into educational practice. Yet a dispassionate review of the relevant research literature shows that discovery-based practice is not as effective as guided discovery."[48]: 18 

Mayer's point is that people often misuse constructivism to promote pure discovery-based teaching techniques. He proposes that the instructional design recommendations of constructivism are too often aimed at discovery-based practice.[48] Sweller (1988) found evidence that practice by novices during early schema acquisition, distracts these learners with unnecessary search-based activity, when the learner's attention should be focused on understanding (acquiring schemas).[49]

The study by Kirschner et al. from which the quote at the beginning of this section was taken has been widely cited and is important for showing the limits of minimally-guided instruction.[60] Hmelo-Silver et al. responded,[61] pointing out that Kirschner et al. conflated constructivist teaching techniques such as inquiry learning with "discovery learning". (See the preceding two sections of this article.) This would agree with Mayer's viewpoint that even though constructivism as a theory and teaching techniques incorporating guidance are likely valid applications of this theory, nevertheless a tradition of misunderstanding has led to some question "pure discovery" techniques.

The math wars and discovery-based teaching techniques[edit]

The math wars controversy in the United States is an example of the type of heated debate that sometimes follows the implementation of constructivist-inspired curricula in schools. In the 1990s, mathematics textbooks based on new standards largely informed by constructivism were developed and promoted with government support. Although constructivist theory does not require eliminating instruction entirely, some textbooks seemed to recommend this extreme. Some parents and mathematicians protested the design of textbooks that omitted or de-emphasized instruction of standard mathematical methods. Supporters responded that the methods were to be eventually discovered under direction by the teacher, but since this was missing or unclear, many insisted the textbooks were designed to deliberately eliminate instruction of standard methods. In one commonly adopted text, the standard formula for the area of a circle is to be derived in the classroom, but not actually printed in the student textbook as is explained by the developers of CMP: "The student role of formulating, representing, clarifying, communicating, and reflecting on ideas leads to an increase in learning. If the format of the texts included many worked examples, the student role would then become merely reproducing these examples with small modifications."[62]

Similarly, this approach has been applied to reading with whole language and inquiry-based science that emphasizes the importance of devising rather than just performing hands-on experiments as early as the elementary grades (traditionally done by research scientists), rather than studying facts. In other areas of curriculum such as social studies and writing are relying more on "higher order thinking skills" rather than memorization of dates, grammar or spelling rules or reciting correct answers. Advocates of this approach counter that the constructivism does not require going to extremes, that in fact teachable moments should regularly infuse the experience with the more traditional teaching. The primary differentiation from the traditional approach being that the engagement of the students in their learning makes them more receptive to learning things at an appropriate time, rather than on a preset schedule.

Importance of structure in constructivist learning environments[edit]

During the 1990s, several theorists began to study the cognitive load of novices (those with little or no prior knowledge of the subject matter) during problem solving. Cognitive load theory was applied in several contexts.[63][64][65][66][57][67] Based on the results of their research, these authors do not support the idea of allowing novices to interact with ill-structured learning environments. Ill-structured learning environments rely on the learner to discover problem solutions. Jonassen (1997) also suggested that novices be taught with "well-structured" learning environments.[68]

Jonassen (1997) also proposed well-designed, well-structured learning environments provide scaffolding for problem-solving. Finally, both Sweller and Jonassen support problem-solving scenarios for more advanced learners.[68][69]

Sweller and his associates even suggest well-structured learning environments, like those provided by worked examples, are not effective for those with more experience—this was later described as the "expertise reversal effect".[52] Cognitive load theorists suggest worked examples initially, with a gradual introduction of problem solving scenarios; this is described as the "guidance fading effect"[58][70] Each of these ideas provides more evidence for Anderson's ACT-R framework.[71] This ACT-R framework suggests learning can begin with studying examples.

Finally Mayer states: "Thus, the contribution of psychology is to help move educational reform efforts from the fuzzy and unproductive world of educational ideology—which sometimes hides under the banner of various versions of constructivism—to the sharp and productive world of theory-based research on how people learn."[48]: 18 

Confusion between constructivist and maturationist views[edit]

Many people confuse constructivist with maturationist views. The constructivist (or cognitive-developmental) stream "is based on the idea that the dialectic or interactionist process of development and learning through the student's active construction should be facilitated and promoted by adults".[72] Whereas, "The romantic maturationist stream is based on the idea that the student's naturally occurring development should be allowed to flower without adult interventions in a permissive environment."[72] In other words, adults play an active role in guiding learning in constructivism, while they are expected to allow children to guide themselves in maturationism.

Subtypes[edit]

Contextual constructivism[edit]

According to William Cobern (1991) Contextual constructivism is "about understanding the fundamental, culturally based beliefs that both students and teachers bring to class, and how these beliefs are supported by culture. Contextual constructivists not only raise new research questions, they also call for a new research paradigm. The focus on contextualization means that qualitative, especially ethnographic, techniques are to be preferred" (p. 3).[73]

Radical constructivism[edit]

Ernst von Glasersfeld developed radical constructivism by coupling Piaget's theory of learning and philosophical viewpoint about the nature of knowledge with Kant's rejection of an objective reality independent of human perception or reason. Radical constructivism does not view knowledge as an attempt to generate ideas that match an independent, objective reality.[74] Instead, theories and knowledge about the world, as generated by our senses and reason, either fit within the constraints of whatever reality may exist and, thus, are viable or do not and are not viable.[75] As a theory of education, radical constructivism emphasizes the experiences of the learner, differences between learners and the importance of uncertainty.[76]

Relational constructivism[edit]

Björn Kraus' relational constructivism can be perceived as a relational consequence of radical constructivism. In contrast to social constructivism, it picks up the epistemological threads and maintains the radical constructivist idea that humans cannot overcome their limited conditions of reception. Despite the subjectivity of human constructions of reality, relational constructivism focuses on the relational conditions that apply to human perceptional processes.[77]

Social constructivism[edit]

In recent decades, constructivist theorists have extended the traditional focus on individual learning to address collaborative and social dimensions of learning. It is possible to see social constructivism as a bringing together of aspects of the work of Piaget with that of Bruner and Vygotsky.[78]

Communal constructivism[edit]

The concept Communal constructivism was developed by Leask and Younie[79] in 1995 through their research on the European SchoolNet[80] which demonstrated the value of experts collaborating to push the boundaries of knowledge i.e. communal construction of new knowledge between experts rather than social construction of knowledge as described by Vygotsky where there is a learner to teacher scaffolding relationship. "Communal constructivism" as a concept applies to those situations in which there is currently no expert knowledge or research to underpin knowledge in an area. "Communal constructivism" refers specifically to the process of experts working together to create, record and publish new knowledge in emerging areas. In the seminal European SchoolNet research where for the first time academics were testing out how the internet could support classroom practice and pedagogy, experts from a number of countries set up test situations to generate and understand new possibilities for educational practice.

Bryan Holmes in 2001 applied this to student learning as described in an early paper, "in this model, students will not simply pass through a course like water through a sieve but instead leave their own imprint in the learning process."[81]

Influence on computer science and robotics[edit]

Constructivism has influenced the course of programming and computer science. Some famous programming languages have been created, wholly or in part, for educational use, to support the constructionist theory of Seymour Papert. These languages have been dynamically typed, and reflective. Logo and its successor Scratch are the best known of them. Constructivism has also informed the design of interactive machine learning systems,[82] whereas Radical Constructivism has been explored as a paradigm to design experiments in rehabilitation robotics, more precisely in prosthetics.[83]

List of notable constructivists[edit]

Writers who influenced constructivism include:

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ White, Fiona Ann; Hayes, Brett Kenneth; Livesey, David James (2016). Developmental Psychology: From Infancy to Adulthood (4th ed.). Melbourne, Vic.: Pearson Australia. ISBN 9781486018277. OCLC 904034548.
  2. ^ Bjorklund, David F. (1 November 2018). "A Metatheory for Cognitive Development (or "Piaget is Dead" Revisited)". Child Development. 89 (6): 2288–2302. doi:10.1111/cdev.13019. PMID 29336015. Archived from the original on 14 August 2021. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  3. ^ Steffe, Leslie P.; Gale, Jerry (2012). Constructivism in Education. Oxon: Routledge. ISBN 978-1-136-47608-2.
  4. ^ a b Seifert, Kelvin & Sutton, Rosemary. Educational Psychology: Second Edition Archived 29 August 2017 at the Wayback Machine. Global Text Project, 2009, pp. 33–37.
  5. ^ Piaget, J., Psychology and Epistemology: Towards a Theory of Knowledge (New York: Grossman, 1971).
  6. ^ Lave, Jean; Wenger, Etienne (27 September 1991). Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-42374-8.[page needed]
  7. ^ a b c d Brown, J.S.; Collins, A.; Duguid, P. (1989). "Situated cognition and the culture of learning". Educational Researcher. 18 (1): 32–42. doi:10.3102/0013189x018001032. hdl:2142/17979. S2CID 9824073.[permanent dead link]
  8. ^ Newman, Denis; Griffin, Peg; Cole, Michael (28 April 1989). The Construction Zone: Working for Cognitive Change in School. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-38942-6.[page needed]
  9. ^ Rogoff, Barbara (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: cognitive development in social context. Oxford University Press.[page needed]
  10. ^ Eddy, Matthew Daniel (2004). "Fallible or Inerrant? A Belated review of the "Constructivist Bible"". British Journal for the History of Science. 37: 93–8. doi:10.1017/s0007087403005338. S2CID 141028650. Archived from the original on 14 August 2021. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  11. ^ Carey, Susan; Zaitchik, Deborah; Bascandziev, Igor (1 December 2015). "Theories of development: In dialog with Jean Piaget". Developmental Review. 38: 36–54. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.003. ISSN 0273-2297.
  12. ^ Tobias, S.; Duffy, T. M. (2009). Constructivist instruction: Success or failure?. New York: Taylor & Francis. ISBN 9780415994231.
  13. ^ a b Wertsch, James V. (1997). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Univ. Press. OCLC 489891986.
  14. ^ a b Von Glasersfeld, Ernst (1998). "Cognition, Construction of Knowledge, and Teaching" (PDF). Constructivism in Science Education. Springer, Dordrecht. pp. 11–30. ISBN 978-0-7923-4924-2. Archived (PDF) from the original on 16 May 2017. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  15. ^ a b Prawat, Richard S.; Floden, Robert E. (1 January 1994). "Philosophical perspectives on constructivist views of learning". Educational Psychologist. 29 (1): 37–48. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2901_4. ISSN 0046-1520. Archived from the original on 14 August 2021. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  16. ^ a b c d e Vygotsky, L. S.; Cole, Michael (1978). Mind in Society: Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-57629-2.
  17. ^ Bauersfeld, H. (1995). "'Language games' in the mathematics classroom: Their function and their effects". In Cobb, Paul; Bauersfeld, H. (eds.). The emergence of mathematical meaning: interaction in classroom cultures. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-1728-X. OCLC 31436489.
  18. ^ Gamoran, Adam; Secada, Walter G.; Marrett, Cora B. (2000). "The Organizational Context of Teaching and Learning" (PDF). In Hallinan, M.T. (ed.). Handbook of the Sociology of Education. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Springer, Boston, MA. pp. 37–63. ISBN 978-0-387-32517-0. Archived (PDF) from the original on 31 January 2018. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  19. ^ a b Brownstein, Bonnie (22 December 2001). "Collaboration: the foundation of learning in the future". Education. 122 (2).
  20. ^ Rhodes, Lynn K.; Bellamy, G. Thomas (1 January 1999). "Choices and Consequences in the Renewal of Teacher Education". Journal of Teacher Education. 50 (1): 17–26. doi:10.1177/002248719905000103. ISSN 0022-4871. S2CID 143182193.
  21. ^ a b c Di Vesta, Francis J. (1987). "The Cognitive Movement and Education". Historical Foundations of Educational Psychology. Perspectives on Individual Differences. Boston: Springer. pp. 203–233. doi:10.1007/978-1-4899-3620-2_11. ISBN 978-1-4899-3622-6.
  22. ^ a b Holt, Dan G.; Willard-Holt, Colleen (1 November 2000). "Let's Get Real™: Students Solving Authentic Corporate Problems". Phi Delta Kappan. 82 (3): 243–246. doi:10.1177/003172170008200315. ISSN 0031-7217. S2CID 143466659.
  23. ^ a b c d McMahon, M. (December 1997). Social Constructivism and the World Wide Web – A Paradigm for Learning. Australasian Society for Computers in Learning in Tertiary Education 1997 conference. Vol. 327. Perth.
  24. ^ a b Savery, Lawson K. (1 June 1994). "The Influence of the Perceived Styles of Leadership on a Group of Workers on their Attitudes to Work". Leadership & Organization Development Journal. 15 (4): 12–18. doi:10.1108/01437739410059863. ISSN 0143-7739.
  25. ^ Archee, Ray; Hill Duin, DA (1995). The World Wide Web and Distance Education: Congergenece or Cacophony?. AUUG Conference Proceedings. AUUG, Inc. pp. 348–356. Archived from the original on 14 August 2021. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  26. ^ Bruning, Roger H.; Schraw, Gregory J.; Ronning, Royce R. (1999). Cognitive Psychology and Instruction (3rd ed.). Prentice-Hall, Inc. ISBN 978-0-13-716606-0.
  27. ^ Meter, Peggy Van; Stevens, Robert J. (1 January 2000). "The Role of Theory in the Study of Peer Collaboration". The Journal of Experimental Education. 69 (1): 113–127. doi:10.1080/00220970009600652. ISSN 0022-0973. S2CID 143292199.
  28. ^ Greeno, James G; Collins, Allan M; Resnick, Lauren B (1996). "Cognition and learning". Handbook of Educational Psychology. 77: 15–46.
  29. ^ a b c d Ackerman, Phillip L. (1 March 1996). "A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge". Intelligence. 22 (2): 227–257. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90016-1. ISSN 0160-2896.
  30. ^ Hsu, Liwei (2013). "English as a foreign language learners' perception of mobile assisted language learning: a cross-national study". Computer Assisted Language Learning. 26 (3). Taylor & Francis online: 197–213. doi:10.1080/09588221.2011.649485. S2CID 62711257.
  31. ^ Ernest, Paul (1991). The philosophy of mathematics education. London: Falmer Press. ISBN 0-203-22423-X. OCLC 52100009.
  32. ^ a b c Duffy, Thomas; Jonassen, eds. (1992). Constructivism and the Technology of Instruction: A Conversation. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 978-0-8058-1272-5.[page needed]
  33. ^ Derry, Sharon J. (2014). "A Fish Called Peer Learning: Searching for Common Themes". In O'Donnell, Angela M.; King, Alison (eds.). Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning. Hoboken: Taylor and Francis. ISBN 978-1-4106-0371-5. OCLC 881162577.
  34. ^ Woolfolk, Anita (2010). Educational psychology (11th ed.). Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Merrill. ISBN 978-0-13-714454-9. OCLC 268547518.
  35. ^ a b Hmelo-Silver; Duncan; Chinn (2007). "Scaffolding and Achievement in Problem-Based and Inquiry Learning: A Response to Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006)" (PDF). Educational Psychologist. 42 (2): 99–107. doi:10.1080/00461520701263368. S2CID 1360735. Archived from the original (PDF) on 23 December 2010. Retrieved 27 December 2007.
  36. ^ Guthrie; et al. (2004). "Increasing Reading Comprehension and Engagement Through Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction" (PDF). Journal of Educational Psychology. 96 (3): 403–423. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.3.403. Archived from the original (PDF) on 25 May 2006. Retrieved 29 December 2007.
  37. ^ Kim (2005). "The Effects of a Constructivist Teaching Approach on Student Academic Achievement, Self-Concept, and Learning Strategies" (PDF). Asia Pacific Education Review. 6 (1): 7–19. doi:10.1007/bf03024963. S2CID 13864166. Archived (PDF) from the original on 6 March 2009. Retrieved 16 December 2007.
  38. ^ Doğru; Kalender (2007). "Applying the Subject 'Cell' Through Constructivist Approach during Science Lessons and the Teacher's View" (PDF). Journal of Environmental & Science Education. 2 (1): 3–13. Archived (PDF) from the original on 6 March 2009. Retrieved 16 December 2007.
  39. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Kirschner, P. A.; Sweller, J.; Clark, R. E. (2006). "Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: an analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching". Educational Psychologist. 41 (2): 75–86. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1. hdl:1820/8951. S2CID 17067829. Archived from the original on 14 August 2021. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  40. ^ Anderson, John R.; Reder, Lynne M.; Simon, Herbert A. (1998). Applications and misapplications of cognitive psychology in mathematics education. Archived from the original on 8 September 2011. Retrieved 4 February 2007.
  41. ^ Constructivism in Science and Mathematics Education Archived 18 November 2009 at the Wayback Machine, Michael R. Matthews
  42. ^ Research Link / Caution: Constructivism Ahead Archived 27 April 2006 at the Wayback Machine Holloway, Educational Leadership, 57 (3). November 1999.
  43. ^ Liu, Charlotte Hua; Matthews, Robert (9 July 2005). "Vygotsky's Philosophy: Constructivism and Its Criticisms Examined" (PDF). International Education Journal. 6 (3): 386–99. ISSN 1443-1475. ERIC EJ854992. Archived (PDF) from the original on 9 August 2021.
  44. ^ Matthews, Michael R. (1993). "Constructivism and science education: Some epistemological problems". Journal of Science Education and Technology. 2 (1). Springer Science and Business Media LLC: 359–370. Bibcode:1993JSEdT...2..359M. doi:10.1007/bf00694598. ISSN 1059-0145. S2CID 144764423. Archived from the original on 14 August 2021. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  45. ^ Raymond Hubbard; J. Scott Armstrong (2005). "Why We Don't Really Know What "Statistical Significance" Means: A Major Educational Failure*" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 June 2010.
  46. ^ Demetriou, A. (1998). Cognitive development. In A. Demetriou, W. Doise, K. F. M. van Lieshout (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology (pp. 179–269). London: Wiley.
  47. ^ Demetriou, A., Shayer, M., & Efklides, A. (1992). Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development: Implications and applications to education. London: Routledge
  48. ^ a b c d e f g h i Mayer (2004). "Should There Be a Three-Strikes Rule Against Pure Discovery Learning?" (PDF). American Psychologist. 59 (1): 14–19. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.372.2476. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.59.1.14. PMID 14736316. S2CID 1129364. Archived from the original (PDF) on 15 February 2015. Retrieved 29 December 2007.
  49. ^ a b c Sweller, J (June 1988). "Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning". Cognitive Science. 12 (2): 257–285. doi:10.1016/0364-0213(88)90023-7. ISSN 0364-0213.
  50. ^ Slezak, Peter (2010). "Radical Constructivism: Epistemology, Education and Dynamite". Constructivist Foundations. 6 (1). ISSN 1782-348X.
  51. ^ Meyer, D. L. (2009). "The Poverty of Constructivism". Educational Philosophy and Theory. 41 (3): 332–341. doi:10.1111/j.1469-5812.2008.00457.x. S2CID 144604333.
  52. ^ a b c Kalyuga, S.; Ayres, P.; Chandler, P.; Sweller, J. (2003). "The expertise reversal effect". Educational Psychologist. 38 (1): 23–31. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3801_4. S2CID 10519654. Archived from the original on 10 November 2015. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  53. ^ Atkinson, R. K.; Derry, S. J.; Renkl, A.; Wortham, D. W. (2000). "Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research". Review of Educational Research. 70 (2): 181–214. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.115.1348. doi:10.3102/00346543070002181. S2CID 2956761.
  54. ^ Hilbert, T. S.; Renkl, A. (2007). "Learning how to Learn by Concept Mapping: A Worked-Example Effect". Paper Presentation at the 12th Biennial Conference EARLI 2007 in Budapest, Hungary.
  55. ^ Tarmizi, R. A.; Sweller, J. (1988). "Guidance during mathematical problem solving". Journal of Educational Psychology. 80 (4): 424–436. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.4.424. hdl:1959.4/69310. Archived from the original on 4 November 2020. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  56. ^ Gerjets, P.; Scheiter, K.; Catrambone, R. (2004). "Designing instructional examples to reduce intrinsic cognitive load: molar versus modular presentation of solution procedures" (PDF). Instructional Science. 32 (1): 33–58. doi:10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021809.10236.71. S2CID 16755228. Archived (PDF) from the original on 27 February 2021. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  57. ^ a b Sweller, J.; Cooper, G. A. (1985). "The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra". Cognition and Instruction. 2 (1): 59–89. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0201_3.
  58. ^ a b Renkl, A.; Atkinson, R. K.; Maier, U. H.; Staley, R. (2002). "From example study to problem solving: Smooth transitions help learning". Journal of Experimental Education. 70 (4): 293–315. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.464.2351. doi:10.1080/00220970209599510. S2CID 21032460.
  59. ^ Sweller, John (1999). Instructional Design in Technical Areas. Australian education review. Camberwell: ACER Press. ISBN 978-0-86431-312-6.
  60. ^ Nilson, Linda Burzotta (2010). Teaching at Its Best: A Research-Based Resource for College Instructors. San Francisco: John Wiley and Sons. p. 176. ISBN 9780470401040.
  61. ^ Hmelo-Silver, Cindy E.; Ravit Golan Duncan; Clark A. Chinn (2007). "Scaffolding and Achievement in Problem-Based and Inquiry Learning: A Response to Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006)". Educational Psychologist. 42 (2): 99–107. doi:10.1080/00461520701263368. S2CID 1360735.
  62. ^ "CMP2 Parent Website FAQ". Archived from the original on 22 June 2009. Retrieved 8 January 2009.
  63. ^ Paas, Fred G. (1992). "Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach". Journal of Educational Psychology. 84 (4): 429–434. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.429. ISSN 1939-2176.
  64. ^ Moreno, Roxana; Mayer, Richard E. (1999). "Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The role of modality and contiguity" (PDF). Journal of Educational Psychology. 91 (2): 358–368. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.458.4719. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.2.358. ISSN 0022-0663. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 August 2017. Retrieved 13 February 2020.
  65. ^ Mousavi, Seyed Yaghoub; Low, Renae; Sweller, John (1995). "Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes". Journal of Educational Psychology. 87 (2): 319–334. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.471.2089. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.87.2.319. ISSN 0022-0663.
  66. ^ Chandler, Paul; Sweller, John (June 1992). "The split-attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction". British Journal of Educational Psychology. 62 (2): 233–246. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1992.tb01017.x. ISSN 0007-0998. S2CID 40723362.
  67. ^ Cooper, Graham; Sweller, John (1987). "Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer". Journal of Educational Psychology. 79 (4): 347–362. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.79.4.347. ISSN 0022-0663.
  68. ^ a b Jonassen, David H. (March 1997). "Instructional design models for well-structured and III-structured problem-solving learning outcomes". Educational Technology Research and Development. 45 (1): 65–94. doi:10.1007/BF02299613. ISSN 1042-1629. S2CID 18701133.
  69. ^ Kalyuga, Slava; Ayres, Paul; Chandler, Paul; Sweller, John (1 January 2003). "The Expertise Reversal Effect". Educational Psychologist. 38 (1). Informa UK Limited: 23–31. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3801_4. ISSN 0046-1520. S2CID 10519654.
  70. ^ Sweller, J (2003). "Evolution of human cognitive architecture". In Ross, Brian (ed.). Psychology of Learning and Motivation. San Diego: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-543343-3. Archived from the original on 14 August 2017. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  71. ^ Clark, R. E. & Elen, J., (2006). When less is more: Research and theory insights about instruction for complex learning. In R. E. Clark & J. Elen (Eds.) Handling Complexity in Learning Environments: Research and Theory. London: Elsevier. 283–295.
  72. ^ a b DeVries, Rheta, ed. (2002). Developing constructivist early childhood curriculum: practical principles and activities. Early childhood education series. New York: Teachers College Press. ISBN 978-0-8077-4121-4.
  73. ^ Cobern, William (April 1991). "Contextual Constructivism: The Impact of Culture on the Learning and Teaching of Science". Theoretical Bases for Science Education Research.
  74. ^ von Glasersfeld, Ernst (1995). Radical Constructivism: A Way of Knowing and Learning. Studies in Mathematics Education Series, no. 6. London: Falmer Press. ISBN 0-7507-0387-3. OCLC 52130078. ERIC ED381352.
  75. ^ von Glasersfeld, Ernst (1990). "Chapter 2: An Exposition of Constructivism: Why Some Like It Radical". Journal for Research in Mathematics Education: Monograph. 4. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics: 19–29, 195–210. doi:10.2307/749910. ISSN 0883-9530. JSTOR 749910.
  76. ^ Gash, H. (15 July 2014). "Constructing Constructivism". Constructivist Foundations. 9 (3): 302–310.
  77. ^ See Björn Kraus: The Life We Live and the Life We Experience: Introducing the Epistemological Difference between "Lifeworld" (Lebenswelt) and "Life Conditions" (Lebenslage) Social Work and Society. International Online Journal. Vol. 13, No. 2 2015, http://www.socwork.net/sws/article/view/438 Archived 13 April 2019 at the Wayback Machine; Björn Kraus: Plädoyer für den Relationalen Konstruktivismus und eine Relationale Soziale Arbeit. in Forum Sozial (2017) 1 pp. 29–35, http://www.ssoar.info/ssoar/handle/document/51948 Archived 1 August 2020 at the Wayback Machine
  78. ^ Wood, David (1998). How Children Think and Learn. Understanding children's worlds (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. pp. 39. ISBN 978-0-631-20007-9.
  79. ^ Leask, M., and Younie, S. (2001a) 'Communal Constructivist Theory: pedagogy of information and communications technology & internationalisation of the curriculum', Journal of Information Technology for Teacher Education, Vol. 10, Nos 1 & 2, pp117 –134
  80. ^ Younie, S.; Leask, M. (2001b). "The European SchoolNet: An online community for European teachers? A valuable professional resource?". Teacher Development. 5 (2): 157–172. doi:10.1080/13664530100200140. S2CID 145109452. Archived from the original on 15 August 2016. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  81. ^ Holmes, Bryan; Tangney, Brendan; FitzGibbon, Ann; Savage, Tim; Mehan, Siobhan. "Communal Constructivism: Students constructing learning for as well as with others" (PDF). Trinity College. Archived (PDF) from the original on 27 February 2021. Retrieved 14 August 2021.
  82. ^ Sarkar, Advait (1 January 2016). Constructivist Design for Interactive Machine Learning. Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems. New York: ACM. pp. 1467–1475. doi:10.1145/2851581.2892547. ISBN 9781450340823. S2CID 1949678.
  83. ^ Nowak, Markus; Castellini, Claudio; Massironi, Carlo (2018). "Applying Radical Constructivism to machine learning: a pilot study in assistive robotics". Constructivist Foundations. 13 (2): 250–262. Archived from the original on 21 February 2019. Retrieved 20 February 2019.

Further reading[edit]

External links[edit]